This website uses cookies

We use cookies and website tracking tools to provide you with the best online experience. Learn more in our privacy statement.

if you accept, your choice will be valid until cancellation.

If you disallow cookies, functionality of our website might be limited.

 

Press releases

Give and take: how we recognize interactions

Tübingen neuroscientists investigate how actions that relate be-tween individuals are recognised and represented in the brain

Social behaviour relies on interactions with others. How does our brain perceive and process these interactions? Until recently, psychologists and neuroscientists have mainly investigated the neuronal processing of individual actions. A team of neuroscientists of three Tübingen research institutes has now shown that social interactions are represented as pairs of actions of the interacting partners in the human brain. The study, whose results may influence future autism research, will be published in PNAS.

Dancing, shaking hands, playing ballgames – all activities of this kind rely on interconnected action pairs of at least two persons, such as „giving and taking“ or „throwing and catching“. How does the brain process interactions in which the actions of several individuals are interrelated? Do we recognise interactions such as these by correlating the individual partners’ motions on a very basic, perception-based level? Or do we first analyse their actions cognitively and attribute a meaning to them?

A collaborative study of neuroscientists belonging to the Tübingen Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics (MPI BC), the Hertie Institute for Clinical Brain Research (HIH) and the Werner Reichardt Centre for Integrative Neuroscience (CIN) has tackled these questions. The researchers utilised of a virtual reality environment, in which a life-sized three-dimensional animated avatar performed various actions: some clearly recognisable as parts of an action pair, others action mixes in between. The researchers exploited an adaptation effect: unclear stimuli will be differently interpreted based on what stimuli had been in effect earlier. For instance, if probands are repeatedly shown a “giving” gesture and are subsequently confronted with an unclear mix of a “giving” and “throwing”, they will interpret the action mix as “throwing” in the majority of trials. 

The researchers were intrigued to not only find this adaptation effect with regard to the first part of a social action sequence, such as “giving”, or “throwing”. The effect also occurred when the matching second part of the sequence was shown first: “taking” or “catching” in these instances. It appears that the very neurons responsible for representing “giving” also respond to its matching opposite “taking”. The action pair “giving-taking” is represented as one unit in the brain. Further control experiments showed that only those actions which are part of a matching pair will trigger the above-mentioned adaptation effect, while completely different actions such as “dancing” will not.

“In the human brain, giving and taking are represented together”, affirms Stephan de la Rosa fro, the MPI for Biological Cybernetic, who conceived the study and performed the experiments together with Leonid Fedorov (HIH/CIN). “Our results show that there are neurons that very likely respond similarly to both halves of an action pair”, says Fedorov’s supervisor Martin Giese (HIH/CIN). Concerning the impact of their findings, the authors are optimistic: “In many autistic disorders, the perception of social interactions such as those we have investigated here is impaired. We believe that our results may be an important step on the way to a better understanding of these socio-cognitive disorders.”

 

In the study, the sequence of actions such as 'catch' (left) and 'take' (right) was morphed, i.e. offset against each other. The researchers took advantage of the effect that the perception of a morph (middle left, middle right) changes after either 'taking' or' catching' was considered longer. Image: S. de la Rosa

  

Short interview about the publication

http://www.kyb.tuebingen.mpg.de/press-news-and-events/meet-your-scientist/stephan-de-la-rosa.html

 

Publication

Leonid Fedorov, Dong-Seon Chang, Martin Giese, Heinrich Bülthoff, Stephan de la Rosa: Adaptation Aftereffects Reveal Representations for Encoding of Contingent Social Actions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (in press)
DOI 10.1073/pnas.1801364115
 

Author contact 

Dr. Stephan de la Rosa
Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics 
Tel.: +49 (0)7071 601 606
delarosa@tuebingen.mpg.de 

Prof. Dr. Martin Giese
Hertie Institute for Clinical Brain Research (HIH) / Werner Reichardt Centrum Centre for Integrative Neuroscience (CIN)
Tel.: +49 7071 29-89124
martin.giese@uni-tuebingen.de

 

Press contact 

Dr. Mareike Kardinal
Director of Communications
Hertie Institute for Clinical Brain Research
Otfried-Müller-Str. 27
72076 Tübingen

Tel.: +49 7071 29-88800
Email: mareike.kardinal@medizin.uni-tuebingen.de

 

Press release as PDF file

Press contact
Dr. Mareike Kardinal (in parental leave) mareike.kardinal@medizin.uni-tuebingen.de Address

Hertie Institute for Clinical Brain Research
Director of Communications

Otfried-Müller-Str. 27
72076 Tübingen

 

For media questions, please contact from now on:

Dr. Hildegard Kaulen
Karlsruher Str. 8
65205 Wiesbaden

Phone: 06122 52718
Email: h.k@kaulen-wissenschaft.de